Ackerman, T., & Ellis, A. W. (2007). Case study: Where do aphasic perseverations come from? Aphasiology, 21(10-11), 1018-1038. doi: 10.1080/02687030701198361
Adlam, A. L., Patterson, K., Bozeat, S., & Hodges, J. R. (2010). The Cambridge Semantic Memory Test Battery: detection of semantic deficits in semantic dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neurocase, 16(3), 193-207. doi: 10.1080/13554790903405693
Alajouanine, T. (1956). Verbal realisation in aphasia. Brain, 79, 1–28.
Baars, B. J., Motley, M. T., & MacKay, D. G. (1975) Output editing for lexical status in artificially elicited slips of the tongue. Journal of Verbal Learning and Behaviour, 14, 382-391.
Balota, D., Yap, M., J., Hutchinson, K. A., Cortese, M. J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely., J. H., Nelson, D. L., Simpson, G. B., & Treiman, R. (2007). Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445-459. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193014.
Baron, J., & Strawson, C. (1976). Use of orthographic and word-specific knowledge in reading words aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2(3), 386-393. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.2.3.386
Bayles, K. A., Tomoeda, C. K., Kaszniak, A. W., Stern, L. Z., & Eagans, K. K. (1985). Verbal perseveration of dementia patients. Brain and Language, 25(1), 102-116.
Binder, J. R. (2017). Current Controversies on Wernicke's Area and its Role in Language. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep, 17(8), 58. doi: 10.1007/s11910-017-0764-8
Blumstein, S. E., Cooper, W. E., Goodglass, H., Statlender, S., & Gottlieb, J. (1980). Production deficits in aphasia: A voice-onset time analysis. Brain and Language, 9(2), 153-170. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(80)90137-6
Bose, A., van Lieshout, P., & Squares, P. A. (2007). Word frequency and bigram frequency effects on linguistic processing and speech motor performance in individuals with aphasia and normal speakers. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(1), 65-88, doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.05.001
Brown, J. W. (1981). Chapter 1 - Introduction Jargonaphasia (pp. 1-8): New York:Academic Press.
Brown, J. W. (1981). Chapter 9 – Case Reports of Semantic Jargon (pp. 169-176): New York:Academic Press.
Buchsbaum, B. R., Baldo, J., Okada, K., Berman, K. F., Dronkers, N., D'Esposito, M., & Hickok, G. (2011). Conduction aphasia, sensory-motor integration, and phonological short-term memory - an aggregate analysis of lesion and fMRI data. Brain and Language, 119(3), 119-128. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.12.001
Buchsbaum,B. R., Hickok, G., & Humphries, C. (2001). Role of left posterior superior temporal gyrus in phonological processing for speech perception and production. Cognitive Science, 25, 663–678. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2505_2
Buckingham, H. W. (1977). The Conduction Theory and Neologistic Jargon. Language and Speech, 20(2), 174–184. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383097702000209
Buckingham, H. W. (1990). Abstruse neologisms, retrieval deficits and the random generator. J Neurolinguistics, 5(2–3), 215-235. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0911-6044(90)90012-N
Buckingham, H. W., & Buckingham, S. S. (2011). Is recurrent perseveration a product of deafferented functional systems with otherwise normal post-activation decay rates? Clin Linguist Phon, 25(11-12), 1066-1073. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2011.616982
Buckingham Jr, H. W., & Kertesz, A. (1974). A linguistic analysis of fluent aphasia. Brain and Language, 1(1), 43-61. doi: http://dx.doi.org.idpproxy.reading.ac.uk/10.1016/0093-934X(74)90025-X
Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area. Neuroimage, 22(1), 466-476, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.049
Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (1998). Competition between past and present. Assessment and interpretation of verbal perseverations. Brain, 121(9), 1641-1659.
Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Vinckier, F., Jobert, A., & Montavont, A. (2008). Reading normal and degraded words: Contribution of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways. Neuroimage, 40, 353:366
Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 33(4), 497-505. doi: 10.1080/14640748108400805
Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed- processing approaches. Psychol Rev, 100(4), 589-608.
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychol Rev, 108(1), 204-256.
Corbett, F., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2008). The use of cueing to alleviate recurrent verbal perseverations: Evidence from transcortical sensory aphasia. Aphasiology, 22(4), 363-382. doi: 10.1080/02687030701415245
Crisp, J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). Unlocking the nature of the phonological-deep dyslexia continuum: the keys to reading aloud are in phonology and semantics. J Cogn Neurosci, 18(3), 348-362. doi: 10.1162/089892906775990543
Dell, G. S., Martin, N., & Schwartz, M. F. (2007). A Case-Series Test of the Interactive Two-step Model of Lexical Access: Predicting Word Repetition from Picture Naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(4), 490-520. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.05.007
Dell, G. S., & Reich, P. A. (1981) Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 20, 611-629.
Dell, G. S., Schwartz, M. F., Martin, N., Saffran, E. M., & Gagnon, D. A. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychol Rev, 104(4), 801-838.
Demeyere N, Riddoch MJ, Slavkova ED, Bickerton W-L, & Humphreys GW. (2015). The Oxford Cognitive Screen (OCS): Validation of a stroke-specific short cognitive screening tool. Psychological Assessment, 27(3), 883–894.
Eaton, E., Marshall, J. & Pring, T. (2011). Mechanisms of change in the evolution of jargon aphasia. Aphasiology, 25(12), pp. 1543-1561. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2011.624584
Fischer-Baum, S., & Rapp, B. (2012). Underlying cause(s) of letter perseveration errors. Neuropsychologia, 50(2), 305-318. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.001
Foygel, D., & Dell, G. S. (2000). Models of Impaired Lexical Access in Speech Production. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(2), 182-216. doi: https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2716
Frankel, T., Penn, C., & Ormond‐Brown, D. (2007). Executive dysfunction as an explanatory basis for conversation symptoms of aphasia: A pilot study. Aphasiology, 21(6-8), 814-828.
Gerhand, S., & Barry, C. (1998). Word frequency effects in oral reading are not merely age-of-acquisition effects in disguise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(2), 267-283. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.24.2.267
Godbold, C. (2016). Non-word errors in jargon aphasia : exploring the underlying mechanisms. Thesis (PhD.) - University of Reading, School of Psychology & Clinical Language Sciences.
Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., & Barresi, B. (2001). The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott.
Gotts, S. J., della Rocchetta, A. I., & Cipolotti, L. (2002). Mechanisms underlying perseveration in aphasia: evidence from a single case study. Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 1930-1947. doi: Pii S0028-3932(02)00067-2Doi 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00067-2
Grainger, J., & Segui, J. (1990). Neighborhood frequency effects in visual word recognition: A comparison of lexical decision and masked identification latencies. Perception & Psychophysics, 47(2), 191-198. doi: 10.3758/BF03205983
Halpern, H. (1965). Effect of Stimulus Variables on Verbal Perseveration of Dysphasic Subjects. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 20(2), 421-429. doi: 10.2466/pms.1965.20.2.421
Hanley, J. R., & Kay, J. R(1997). An Effect of Imageability on the Production of Phonological Errors in Auditory Repetition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(8), 1065-1084. doi: 10.1080/026432997381277
Hanley, J. R., Kay, J., & Edwards, M. (2002). Imageability effects, phonological errors, and the relationship between auditory repetition and picture naming: Implications for models of auditory repetition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19(3), 193-206. doi: 10.1080/02643290143000132
Hillis, A. E. (2007). Aphasia. Neurology, 69(2), 200.
Hillis, A. E., Boatman, D., Hart, J., & Gordon, B. (1999). Making sense out of jargon. Neurology, 53(8), 1813.
Hirsh, K. W. (1998). Perseveration and Activation in Aphasic Speech Production. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15(4), 377-388. doi: 10.1080/026432998381140
Hoffman, P., Jefferies, B., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). Remembering ‘zeal’ but not ‘thing’: Reverse frequency effects as a consequence of deregulated semantic processing. Neuropsychologia, 49(3), 580-584. doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.036
Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Schweickert, R., Brown, G. D., Martin, M., & Stuart, G. (1997). Word-frequency effects on short-term memory tasks: evidence for a redintegration process in immediate serial recall. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 23(5), 1217-1232.
Jefferies, E., Crisp, J., & Ralph, M. A. L. (2006). The impact of phonological or semantic impairment on delayed auditory repetition: Evidence from stroke aphasia and semantic dementia. Aphasiology, 20(9), 963-992. doi: 10.1080/02687030600739398
Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., Jones, R. W., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2009). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in semantic dementia. Neuropsychology, 23(4), 492-499. doi: 10.1037/a0015452
Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1996). Psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia (PALPA): An introduction. Aphasiology, 10(2), 159-180. doi: 10.1080/02687039608248403
Kertesz, A. (1981). Chapter 4 - The Anatomy of Jargon A2 - BROWN, JASON W Jargonaphasia (pp. 63-112): Academic Press.
Kertesz, A., & Benson, D. F. (1970). Neologistic Jargon: A Clinicopathological Study. Cortex, 6(4), 362-386. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(70)80002-8
Kinsbourne, M., & Warrington, E. (1963). Jargon aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 1(1), 27-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(63)90010-1
Kittredge, A. K., Dell, G. S., Verkuilen, J., & Schwartz, M. F. (2008). Where is the effect of frequency in word production? Insights from aphasic picture-naming errors. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25(4), 463-492. doi: 10.1080/02643290701674851
Kohn, S. E., Smith, K. L., & Alexander, M. P. (1996). Differential recovery from impairment to the phonological lexicon. Brain and Language, 52(1), 129-149. doi: 10.1006/brln.1996.0007
Laszlo, S., & Federmeier, K. D. (2007). Better the DVL You Know: Acronyms Reveal the Contribution of Familiarity to Single-Word Reading. Psychological Science, 18(2), 122-126. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01859.x
Marshall, J. (2006). Jargon aphasia: What have we learned? Aphasiology, 20(5), 387-410. doi: 10.1080/02687030500489946
Marshall, J., Chiat, S., Robson, J., & Pring, T. (1996). Calling a salad a federation: an investigation of semantic Jargon. Paper 2, Verbs. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 9, 251-260.
Marshall, J., Pring, T., Chiat, S., & Robson, J. (2001). When Ottoman is Easier than Chair: An Inverse Frequency Effect in Jargon Aphasia. Cortex, 37(1), 33-53. doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70556-2
Marshall, J., Robson, J., Pring, T., & Chiat, S. (1998). Why does monitoring fail in Jargon aphasia? Comprehension, judgement and therapy evidence. Brain and Language, 1(1), 79-107. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1997.1936
Martin, N., & Dell, G. S. (2007). Common mechanisms underlying perseverative and non-perseverative sound and word substitutions. Aphasiology, 21(10-11), 1002-1017. doi: 10.1080/02687030701198346
Martin, N., & Saffran, E. M. (1997). Language and auditory-verbal short-term memory impairments: Evidence for common underlying processes. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(5), 641-682. doi: 10.1080/026432997381402
McCarthy, R., & Warrington, E. K. (1984). A two-route model of speech production. Evidence from aphasia. Brain, 107 ( Pt 2), 463-485.
McCloskey, M., Macaruso, P., & Rapp, B. (2006). Grapheme-to-lexeme feedback in the spelling system: Evidence from a dysgraphic patient. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(2), 278-307. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000518
Mesgarani N., Cheung, C., Johnson, K., & Chang, E. F. (2014). Phonetic feature encoding in human superior temporal gyrus. Science. 343(6174):1006-10. doi: 10.1126/science.1245994.
Monsell, S., Doyle, M. C., & Haggard, P. N. (1989). Effects of frequency on visual word recognition tasks: where are they? J Exp Psychol Gen, 118(1), 43-71.
Moses, M. S., Nickels, L. A., & Sheard, C. (2004). Disentangling the web: Neologistic perseverative errors in jargon aphasia. Neurocase, 10(6), 452-461. doi: 10.1080/13554790490894057
Moses, M. S., Sheard, C., & Nickels, L. A. (2007). Insights into recurrent perseveration errors in aphasia: A case series approach. Aphasiology, 21:10-11, 975-1001, DOI: 10.1080/02687030701198312.
Nickels, L., & Howard, D. (2004). Dissociating Effects of Number of Phonemes, Number of Syllables, and Syllabic Complexity on Word Production in Aphasia: It's the Number of Phonemes that Counts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21(1), 57-78.
Nozari, N., & Dell, G. S. (2013). How damaged brains repeat words: A computational approach. Brain and Language, 126(3), 327-337. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.07.005
Nozari, N., Kittredge, A. K., Dell, G. S., & Schwartz, M. F. (2010). Naming and repetition in aphasia: Steps, routes, and frequency effects. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(4), 541-559. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.08.001
O'Connell, P. F. (1981). Neologistic jargon aphasia: A case report. Brain and Language, 12(2), 292-302. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(81)90020-1
Olson, A., Halloran, E., & Romani, C. (2015). Target/error overlap in jargonaphasia: The case for a one-source model, lexical and non-lexical summation, and the special status of correct responses. Cortex, 73, 158-179. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.028
Olson, A. C., Romani, C., & Halloran, L. (2007). Localizing the deficit in a case of jargonaphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(2), 211-238. doi: 10.1080/02643290601137017
Papagno, C., & Basso, A. (1996). Perseveration in two aphasic patients. Cortex, 32(1), 67-82.
Patterson, K., Graham, N., & Hodges, J. R. (1994). The impact of semantic memory loss on phonological representations. J Cogn Neurosci, 6(1), 57-69. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1994.6.1.57
Pekkala, S., Albert, M. L., Spiro Iii, A., & Erkinjuntti, T. (2008). Perseveration in Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders, 25(2), 109-114.
Pilkington, E., Keidel, J., Kendrick, L. T., Saddy, J. D., Sage, K., & Robson, H. (2017). Sources of Phoneme Errors in Repetition: Perseverative, Neologistic, and Lesion Patterns in Jargon Aphasia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11(225). doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00225
Pilkington, E., Sage, K., Saddy, D. and Robson, H. (2019) What can repetition, reading and naming tell us about Jargon Aphasia? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 49. pp. 45-56. ISSN 0911-6044 doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.08.003
Plaut, D. C., & Kello, C. T. (1999). The emergence of phonology from the interplay of speech comprehension and production: A distributed connectionist approach The emergence of language. (pp. 381-415). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Purcell, R., Lambon-Ralph, M., & Sage, K. (2018). Investigating the language, cognition and self-monitoring abilities of speakers with jargon output. Aphasiology, DOI: 10.1080/02687038.2018.1532070
Rayner, K., & Duffy, S. A. (1986). Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory & Cognition, 14(3), 191-201. doi: 10.3758/BF03197692
Richardson, F. M., Seghier, M. L., Leff, A. P., Thomas, M. S. C., & Price, C. (2011). Multiple Routes from Occipital to Temporal Cortices during Reading. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(22) 8239:8247. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6519-10.2011
Robin, D. A., & Schienberg, S. (1990). Subcortical lesions and aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55(1), 90-100.
Robinson, G. A., Butterworth, B., & Cipolotti, L. (2015). “My Mind Is Doing It All”: No “Brake” to Stop Speech Generation in Jargon Aphasia. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 28(4), 229-241.
Robson, H., Grube, M., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Griffiths, T. D., & Sage, K. (2013). Fundamental deficits of auditory perception in Wernicke's aphasia. Cortex, 49(7), 1808-1822. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.11.012
Robson, H., Pilkington, E., Evans, L., DeLuca, V. and Keidel, J. (2017) Phonological and semantic processing during comprehension in Wernicke’s aphasia: a N400 and Phonological Mapping Negativity study. Neuropsychologia, 100. pp. 144-154. ISSN 0028-3932 doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.04.012
Robson, H., Sage, K. and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2012) Wernicke’s aphasia reflects a combination of acoustic-phonological and semantic control deficits: A case-series comparison of Wernicke’s aphasia, semantic dementia and semantic aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 50 (2). pp. 266-275. ISSN 0028-3932 doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.11.021
Robson, H., Specht, K., Beaumont, H., Parkes, L. M., Sage, K., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Zahn, R. (2017). Arterial spin labelling shows functional depression of non-lesion tissue in chronic Wernicke’s aphasia. Cortex, 92: 249–260.
Robson, J., Pring, T., Marshall, J., & Chiat, S. (2003). Phoneme frequency effects in jargon aphasia: a phonological investigation of nonword errors. Brain and Language, 85(1), 109-124.
Romani, C., Galluzzi, C., & Olson, A. (2011). Phonological–lexical activation: A lexical component or an output buffer? Evidence from aphasic errors. Cortex, 47(2), 217-235. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.004
Rorden, C, & Brett, M. (2000) Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behavioural Neurology 12(4), 191-200.
Sampson, M., & Faroqi-Shah, Y. (2011). Investigation of self-monitoring in fluent aphasia with jargon. Aphasiology. 25:4, 505-528, doi: 10.1080/02687038.2010.523471
Sandson, J., & Albert, M. L. (1984). Varieties of perseveration. Neuropsychologia, 22(6), 715-732.
Sandson, J., & Albert, M. L. (1987). Perseveration in behavioral neurology. Neurology, 37(11), 1736-1736.
Santo Pietro, M. J., & Rigrodsky, S. (1986). Patterns of oral-verbal perseveration in adult aphasics. Brain and Language, 29(1), 1-17.
Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., Bloch, D. E., and Dell, G. S. (1994). Disordered Speech Production in Aphasic and Normal Speakers. Brain and Language, 47, 52-88.
Schwartz, M. F., Wilshire, C. E., Gagnon, D. A., & Polansky, M. (2004). Origins of nonword phonological errors in aphasic picture naming. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21(2-4), 159-186. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290342000519
Strain, E., Patterson, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1995). Semantic effects in single-word naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(5), 1140-1154. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.5.1140
Strain, E., Patterson, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2002). Theories of word naming interact with spelling--sound consistency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(1), 207-214. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.28.1.207
Vitevitch, M.S. & Luce, P.A. (2004). A Web-based interface to calculate phonotactic probability for words and nonwords in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 36: 481. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195594
Wang, J., Mathalon, D. J., Roach, B. J., Reilly, J., Keedy, S. K., Sweeny, J. K., & Ford, J. M. (2014). Action planning and predictive coding when speaking. Neuroimage, 91, 91-98. Doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.003
Warrington, E. K., & James, M. (1991). Visual Object and Space Perception Battery (VOSP). Bury St. Edmunds (UK): Thames Valley Test Company
Weekes, B. S. (1997). Differential Effects of Number of Letters on Word and Nonword Naming Latency. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 50(2), 439-456. doi: 10.1080/713755710
Yamadori, A. (1981). Verbal perseveration in aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 19(4), 591-594.