Alloway, T. P. (2015). AWMA-2-NL: Alloway Working Memory Assessment 2nd edition : Nederlandse bewerking. Pearson.
Antoniou, K., Kambanaros, M., Grohmann, K. K., & Katsos, N. (2016). Is Bilectalism Similar to Bilingualism? An Investigation into Children’s Vocabulary and Executive Control Skills. 14.
Antoniou, K., & Spanoudis, G. C. (2020). An Investigation of the Multilingual and Bi-dialectal Advantage in Executive Control. Cognitive Science Society. https://cognitivesciencesociety.org/cogsci20/papers/0470/0470.pdf
Auer, P. (2005). Europe’s sociolinguistic unity, or: A typology of European dialect/standard constellations. In N. Delbecque, J. van der Auwera, & D. Geeraerts (Eds.), Perspectives on Variation. Berlin, New York: DE GRUYTER MOUTON. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110909579.7
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Navarro-Torres, C. A., Dussias, P. E., Bajo, M. T., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Kroll, J. F. (2020). Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(6), 1022–1047. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000770
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., & Titone, D. A. (2021). The quest for signals in noise: Leveraging experiential variation to identify bilingual phenotypes. Languages, 6(4). https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6040168
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, Aging, and Cognitive Control: Evidence From the Simon Task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290–303. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 240–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001
Blanco-Elorrieta, E., & Pylkkänen, L. (2018). Ecological Validity in Bilingualism Research and the Bilingual Advantage. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 1117–1126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.10.001
Blokzijl, J., Deuchar, M., & Couto, M. C. P. (2017). Determiner asymmetry in mixed nominal constructions: The role of grammatical factors in data from Miami and Nicaragua. Languages, 2(4), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2040020
Blom, E., Boerma, T., Bosma, E., Cornips, L., & Everaert, E. (2017). Cognitive Advantages of Bilingual Children in Different Sociolinguistic Contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 552. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00552
Bosma, E., Blom, E., & Versloot, A. (2017a). Language balance and cognitive advantages in Frisian-Dutch bilingual children. In F. Lauchlan & M. del C. P. Couto (Series Eds.), Bilingualism and Minority Languages in Europe: Current Trends and Developments. Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Bosma, E., Hoekstra, E., Versloot, A., & Blom, E. (2017b). The Minimal and Short-Lived Effects of Minority Language Exposure on the Executive Functions of Frisian-Dutch Bilingual Children. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1453. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01453
Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130(3), 278–288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–298. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x
Chen, Z. (2003). Attentional focus, processing load, and Stroop interference. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(6), 888–900. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194822
Dijkstra, J. (2013). Growing up with Frisian and Dutch. 193.
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The Inhibitory Advantage in Bilingual Children Revisited: Myth or Reality? Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 234–251. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000243
Emmorey, K., Luk, G., Pyers, J. E., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The Source of Enhanced Cognitive Control in Bilinguals: Evidence From Bimodal Bilinguals. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1201–1206. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02224.x
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267
Ferguson, C. A. (1959). Diglossia. WORD, 15(2), 325–340. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1959.11659702
Garraffa, M., Beveridge, M., & Sorace, A. (2015). Linguistic and Cognitive Skills in Sardinian–Italian Bilingual Children. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01898
Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Viñas Guasch, N., … Jones, L. (2014). Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00011
Gertken, L. M., Amengual, M., & Birdsong, D. (2014). Assessing language dominance with the bilingual language profile. In P. Leclercq, A. Edmonds, & H. Hilton (Eds.), Measuring L2 proficiency: Perspectives from SLA, . (pp. 208–225).
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013a). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013b). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377
Guerrero, S. L., Mesite, L., Surrain, S., and Luk, G. (2015). Mixed language use and cognitive flexibility in young bilinguals. Retrieved from https://bef2015.commons.gc.cuny.edu/2015/04/30/mixed-language-use-and-cognitive-flexibility-in-young-bilinguals/
Gunnerud, H. L., ten Braak, D., Reikerås, E. K. L., Donolato, E., & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2020). Is bilingualism related to a cognitive advantage in children? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(12), 1059. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000301
Hartanto, A., & Yang, H. (2016). Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition, 150, 10–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.016
Hartanto, A., & Yang, H. (2020). The role of bilingual interactional contexts in predicting interindividual variability in executive functions: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(4), 609–633. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000672
Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Sumner, P. (2018). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods, 50(3), 1166–1186. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1
Hofweber, J., Marinis, T., & Treffers-Daller, J. (2019). Predicting executive functions in bilinguals using ecologically valid measures of code-switching behaviour. In I. A. Sekerina, L. Spradlin, & V. Valian (Eds.), Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond (pp. 181–205). John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Houtzager, N., Lowie, W., Sprenger, S., & De Bot, K. (2017). A bilingual advantage in task switching? Age-related differences between German monolinguals and Dutch-Frisian bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(1), 69–79. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000498
Hsu, H.-L. (2021). Cognitive control in older Minnan-Mandarin and Hakka-Mandarin bidialectal adults: Advantages in Stroop-type tasks. Lingua, 103041. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2021.103041
Kaye, A. S. (2001). Diglossia: The state of the art. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 117–129.
Kirk, N. W., Fiala, L., Scott-Brown, K. C., & Kempe, V. (2014). No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 640–648. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.929580
Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A. (2012). Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(2), 356–369. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.604788
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 497–514. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.799170
Lamb, W. (2008). Scottish Gaelic Speech and Writing: Register Variation in an Endangered Language. 332.
Lauchlan, F., Parisi, M., & Fadda, R. (2013). Bilingualism in Sardinia and Scotland: Exploring the cognitive benefits of speaking a ‘minority’ language. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(1), 43–56. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006911429622
Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., Järvenpää, J., de Bruin, A., & Antfolk, J. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(4), 394–425. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000142
Lowe, C. J., Cho, I., Goldsmith, S. F., & Morton, J. B. (2021). The Bilingual Advantage in Children’s Executive Functioning Is Not Related to Language Status: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychological Science, 32(7), 1115–1146. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797621993108
Luo, L., Craik, F. I. M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 28–34. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030875
MacAulay, D. (Ed.). (1993). The Scottish Gaelic Language. In D. MacAulay, The Celtic Languages. Cambridge England ; New York: Cambridge University Press.
Madau, P., Ferguson, H. C., Dickinson, M. E., Giavalisco, M., Steidel, C. C., & Fruchter, A. (1996). High-redshift galaxies in the Hubble Deep Field: Colour selection and star formation history to z 4. Monthly Notices of the Royal Astronomical Society, 283(4), 1388–1404. https://doi.org/10.1093/mnras/283.4.1388
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 81–93. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907003227
Masica, C. P., & Sinha, A. K. (1986). South Asian Languages: Structure, Convergence and Diglossia : 2nd International Conference on South Asian Languages and Linguistics : Selected Papers. Motilal Banarsidass.
Mejía, A.-M. D. (2002). Power, Prestige, and Bilingualism: International Perspectives on Elite Bilingual Education. Multilingual Matters.
Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., & Ahn, J. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: The effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 115(2–3), 123–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.004
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 187–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10(6), 719–726. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.x
Muller, N., & Ball, M. J. (2005). Code-switching and diglossia. In M. J. Ball (Ed.), Clinical Sociolinguistics (pp. 49–62). Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
Muysken, P. (2000). Bilingual speech: A typology of code-mixing. Cambridge, UK ; New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ooi, S. H., Goh, W. D., Sorace, A., & Bak, T. H. (2018). From Bilingualism to Bilingualisms: Bilingual experience in Edinburgh and Singapore affects attentional control differently. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(4), 867–879. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000020
Oschwald, J., Schättin, A., von Bastian, C. C., & Souza, A. S. (2018). Bidialectalism and Bilingualism: Exploring the Role of Language Similarity as a Link Between Linguistic Ability and Executive Control. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1997. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01997
Paap, K. (2019). The Bilingual Advantage Debate: Quantity and Quality of the Evidence. In J. W. Schwieter & M. Paradis (Eds.), The Handbook of the Neuroscience of Multilingualism (pp. 701–735). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119387725.ch34
Paap, K. R., Anders-Jefferson, R., Zimiga, B., Mason, L., & Mikulinsky, R. (2020). Interference scores have inadequate concurrent and convergent validity: Should we stop using the flanker, Simon, and spatial Stroop tasks? Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 5(1), 7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-020-0207-y
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014
Pavlou, P. (2004). Greek dialect use in the mass media in Cyprus. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 2004(168). https://doi.org/10.1515/ijsl.2004.026
Pelham, S. D., & Abrams, L. (2014). Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 313–325. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035224
Piper, B. J., Li, V., Eiwaz, M. A., Kobel, Y. V., Benice, T. S., Chu, A. M., … Mueller, S. T. (2012). Executive function on the Psychology Experiment Building Language tests. Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 110–123. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0096-6
Pliatsikas, C., DeLuca, V., & Voits, T. (2020). The Many Shades of Bilingualism: Language Experiences Modulate Adaptations in Brain Structure. Language Learning, 70(S2), 133–149. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12386
Poarch, G. J., & Krott, A. (2019). A Bilingual Advantage? An Appeal for a Change in Perspective and Recommendations for Future Research. Behavioral Sciences, 9(9), 95. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9090095
Poarch, G. J., & van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 535–551. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.013
Poarch, G. J., & van Hell, J. G. (2019). Does performance on executive function tasks correlate? In I. A. Sekerina, L. Spradlin, & V. Valian (Eds.), Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and insights (pp. 223–236). John Benjamins Publishing Company. Retrieved from https://benjamins.com/catalog/sibil.57.14poa
Poarch, G. J., Vanhove, J., & Berthele, R. (2019). The effect of bidialectalism on executive function. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(2), 612–628. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006918763132
Pot, A., Keijzer, M., & de Bot, K. (2018). Intensity of Multilingual Language Use Predicts Cognitive Performance in Some Multilingual Older Adults. Brain Sciences, 8(5), 92. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8050092
Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., & Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 567–579. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.009
Ross, J., & Melinger, A. (2017). Bilingual advantage, bidialectal advantage or neither? Comparing performance across three tests of executive function in middle childhood. Developmental Science, 20(4), e12405. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12405
Rouder, J. N., & Haaf, J. M. (2019). A psychometrics of individual differences in experimental tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 26(2), 452–467. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1558-y
Rowe, C., & Grohmann, K. K. (2013). Discrete bilectalism: Towards co-overt prestige and diglossic shift in Cyprus. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 2013(224). https://doi.org/10.1515/ijsl-2013-0058
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Joshi, R. M. (2014). Handbook of Arabic Literacy: Insights and Perspectives. Springer Science & Business Media.
Scaltritti, M., Peressotti, F., & Miozzo, M. (2017). Bilingual advantage and language switch: What’s the linkage? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(1), 80–97. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000565
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
Surrain, S., & Luk, G. (2019). Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015. Bilingualism: Language and
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3–24. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000522
Verhagen, J., Mulder, H., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2017). Effects of home language environment on inhibitory control in bilingual three-year-old children. Bilingualism Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(1), 114–127.
Wechsler, D. & Psychological Corporation. (1949). Wechsler intelligence scale for children: Manual. New York: Psychological Corp.
Yang, H., Hartanto, A., & Yang, S. (2016). The Complex Nature of Bilinguals’ Language Usage Modulates Task-Switching Outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00560